News from Hope College

Happiness is Research about Happiness

[Two women's headshots stylized to look like old polaroids] Dr. Rika Hanamitsu, Professor of Communication at Waseda University in Tokyo and 2018-19 Visiting Professor at Hope. Dr. Deirdre Johnston, Professor of Communication and Interim Associate Dean of Global Studies.

A smile — that universal expression of happiness — usually needs no translation. Upturned corners of a mouth mean “gladness”; add teeth and you have jubilation. Yet, as with most things in human life, it’s not always that simple. What makes one person put on a happy face might make another express ambivalence at best. Add in the multiple ways another culture defines happiness, and you have expressions of glee as wide and varied as the different degrees of that smile.

Dr. Deirdre Johnston, professor of communication and interim associate dean of global studies at Hope, and Dr. Rika Hanamitsu, a professor of communication at Waseda University in Tokyo who served as a visiting professor at Hope during the 2018-19 academic year, have been investigating cross-cultural definitions of happiness between the United States and Japan since 2015. The two met 10 years ago, when Johnston was visiting Tokyo on a Great Lakes Colleges Association trip. Since then, they have collaborated often on teaching and research. Their first project, a multi-institutional, cross-cultural global exposure study in six countries, was published in the journal Intercultural Communication Studies in 2015. “Photovoice Analysis of Cross-Cultural Happiness in Japan and USA” is their second project, and it is uncovering an interesting and glad array of ways to be happy.

In question-and-answer format with interviewer Eva Dean Folkert ’83, here is when the project came to be, how it worked, what the two researchers found and why it matters.

First, you both seem to be happy in each other’s company.
Why do you like conducting research together?

Dede: We generate fun and creative projects together, and we enjoy learning from each other. We each provide a cultural framework to understanding the projects we work on, and this is a great learning opportunity. We also discover and explore cultural biases in existing research, and this speaks to the importance of engaging in cross-cultural research teams. We’ve had a great deal of fun working together as our friendship has grown. We’ve involved a lot of students in the projects as well, and we’ve enjoyed working together with a student research team. Rika is really good at details, like double-checking statistics and making final edits, which I abhor, so it is a great working partnership.

Rika: The greatest part is we’ve developed a long-term friendship through working together.

Why research together on happiness?

Rika: Happiness is a topic that can be researched from a lot of different disciplines, including psychology, sociology, economics, as well as communication studies. There is also a lot of general interest in the topic. It is something we all (across cultures) desire, but there are similarities and differences in how we perceive it, pursue it and experience it. The photography aspect of this project makes it particularly unique and interesting. For me personally, photography is something special because my father was a professional photographer and I grew up with photographs. They capture the moment we tend to miss in daily life as well as the moment we want to record.

Dede: I was intrigued with the happiness lists that started appearing in the media, which ranked countries and states and cities according to their level of happiness. The question that plagued me was: “Do different countries and cultures actually define and experience happiness in the same way?” I thought they might not, making those surveys and rankings spurious. As an empiricist, I wanted to explore it.

So, how did you conduct your study?
It’s quite qualitative in nature.

Rika: Data collection for our project was rather unique. We recruited a stratified sample across four different age groups (18-29, 30-44, 45-65, over 65) in both the United States and Japan. Each participant was given a disposable camera and a booklet with questions. Participants were instructed to conduct the project during a 24-hour period. When they noticed that they were experiencing happiness, they were to pause, take a photograph that represented their happiness at that moment, and answer five narrative prompts reflecting on their happy thoughts. Participants also took a short standardized pre- and post-test happiness assessment at the beginning and end of their 24-hour data collection.

This study has been four years in the making.
What causes a study to take that amount of time?

[They both laugh.]

Dede: It is a very complicated data project with a lot of narrative and photo data. Many qualitative studies have a much smaller sample size, and to code five photos for each of 80 participants, and to code five narrative answers for each of five photos for 80 participants, was a bit overwhelming.

Rika: The topic is vast. And there are many different aspects to this data set which may evolve into multiple publications and possibly a book.

Here, in their combined words, is what they found:

So, why is any of this important, anyway?

Dede: Overall, this study is important because comparisons of countries on the ranking of happiness do not reflect that different cultures may experience, define and contextualize happiness differently. In terms of intercultural communication, when we refer to happiness, we may not be referencing exactly the same experience. We can learn from each other the ways in which each culture may constrain or limit the experience of happiness, and this knowledge could promote greater happiness for all. In addition, the study of cross-cultural happiness promotes intercultural competence through understanding of others’ emotions and experiences, as well as a greater self-awareness.

Exit mobile version